Who Knows Their Children Best, Teachers or Parents? It Depends, Says University of Virginia Researcher

April 10, 2006 — Researchers have generally believed that teachers are better than parents at evaluating the behavior of school children, because teachers have a bigger group of children for comparison. A University of Virginia study, however, shows that parents are better at assessing their child’s emotional states, while teachers are better at rating bad behaviors. The results emphasize the importance of teachers and parents working together in the child’s best interest.

Associate professor Timothy Konold, coordinator of research, statistics and evaluation at U.Va.’s Curry School of Education, reported his findings on April 8 at the annual American Educational Research Association (AERA) meeting in San Francisco.

“Our results indicate that both parent and teacher informants are important considerations when assessing a child’s overall behavioral disposition,” Konold said.

He based his research on ratings given by mothers, fathers and teachers of a representative sample of 562 first-graders in the national NICHD Study of Early Child Care. U.Va. is one of 10 sites of the 15-year research project and is led by Robert Pianta.

A commonly used questionnaire rating 96 behaviors typically reveals different scores on the same behaviors when mothers, fathers and teachers evaluate their children, said Konold, who sought to determine which rating was a better assessment.

“One of the more striking findings is the degree to which parent ratings of children’s internalizing [or emotional] behaviors tap into more of these behaviors than teacher ratings,” he added.

Parents’ scores are better indicators of emotional behaviors, such as being anxious, sad or lonely, or making physical complaints (real or imagined).

“Parents do a much better job of assessing internalizing behaviors, so we should use their ratings when there’s a discrepancy,” he said.

When it comes to aggressive or other delinquent behaviors, called “externalizing,” teachers make a better assessment, he said. Examples of those behaviors include arguing, teasing, threatening, cheating, swearing and lying. These are behaviors children may have learned from their parents, so the parents don’t necessarily recognize them as deviant as consistently as teachers do.

“ The results have important implications for the manner in which we collect information on child behavior problems that are used to inform instruction and counseling decisions,” he said. Konold said researchers have not found these results before because they didn’t have an approach for disentangling the variations in the different environments of school and home. He used a complex methodology developed in the last decade to make it possible for educators to interpret the measurements more effectively.

Tim Konold can be reached by e-mail at konold@virginia.edu or by phone at (434) 924-0824.

Media Contact